Skip to main content

Eyewitness Misidentification

Eyewitness Misidentifications
In our justice system, there are many crucial points where things can go wrong, and an innocent person
can be convicted. Eyewitness testimony is one of those crucial points, which unfortunately happens more
often than we would like to admit. In his book, Garrett focuses on 250 people who were exonerated
through DNA evidence. These 250 people were wrongfully convicted for various reasons, but Garrett
found that eyewitnesses made misidentifications of the perpetrator in 190 of the 250 cases, or 76% of
the cases (Garrett 48, 2012). It is often wondered how a victim could falsely identify the person that
attacked them, if they weren’t wearing a mask, it should be easy to identify your assailant, right? That is
not necessarily true. Of the cases that Garrett studied of those who were wrongfully convicted, he found
a few themes common across them all. For one, Garrett found that in most of the cases, police either
intentionally or unintentionally coerced the eyewitness into picking out a certain suspect from a lineup.
This was done by making suggestive remarks as to who they believe is the perpetrator, by presenting
lineups in a way that made one person stand out more than others, or even conducting a “showup”,
which is when only one person is presented to the witness to make a possible identification. Garrett
found that while most of the eyewitnesses were confident in their identification at the trial, most were
very hesitant about making the identification when first presented with a suspect.
As the video “Eyewitness Testimony” by 60 Minutes pointed out, another crucial problem with
eyewitness testimony is the fragility of memory (Eyewitness Testimony). There are many
factors that take place during an attack that could hinder an eyewitness’ ability to make a
correct identification. As you can imagine, during an attack you might be filled with fear and
adrenaline, and there is too much going on at once to focus on one thing. Things like how
dark it is in the setting, whether the perpetrator was wearing a mask, if the victim was ordered
not to look, or even the witness being focused on a weapon that is present instead of the
perpetrators face, all can hinder the possibility of the witness making a positive identification
(Garret 49, 2012). But even with these factors, witnesses are driven to put the person behind
bars, whatever it takes. This is why, as Garrett points out, witnesses will often choose a person
from a lineup that matches the closest to what they remember the perpetrator looking like.
In order to make eyewitness identifications more accurate, Garrett suggests making lineups
more accurate, in that the suspects shown in the lineup must look somewhat alike, and they
cannot be arranged so one person sticks out more than others. Police should also tell the
witness that the true perpetrator may or may not be present in the lineup, so the witness does
not feel compelled to pick someone in the lineup that looks the closest to the perpetrator. He
suggests that police should also take into consideration the witness’ opportunity to view the
offender, their degree of attention, the passage of time, and cross-racial identifications, when
considering the accuracy of an eyewitness’ identification.


Eyewitness Testimony Part 2 [Video file]. (2012, October 30). Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GtelV9lmzQc

Garrett, B. (2012). Eyewitness Misidentifications. In Convicting the innocent: Where criminal prosecutions go wrong (pp. 45-83). Harvard University Press.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Malcolm Bryant: Exonerated by DNA

Malcolm Bryant: Exonerated by DNA On the evening of November 20, 1998, 17-year-old Tyeisha Powell and 16-year-old Toni Bullock were confronted by a man carrying a knife. They were dragged into a vacant lot in Baltimore, Maryland and Bullock was stabbed to death. Powell managed to escape and later gave police a description of her attacker. She only saw him briefly and other factors such as poor visibility in the rain and evening made it hard for her to give an adequate description. A week later with no leads, police receive a phone call from members of the victim’s family saying a man resembling the sketch was just released from jail. Bryant was picked up and immediately put in a photo lineup where Powell identified him as the attacker. Bryant was charged with first-degree murder, even though witnesses placed him at a nightclub at the time of the crime. Bryant was sentenced to life in prison in 1999. Over the years, Bryant filed motions only to be dismissed and until 2009, ...

David Shawn Pope

Voice Comparison Convicts The Crime In July of 1985 in Dallas County, Texas, a man knocked on a woman’s door asking if somebody lived there, and then immediately left. The following morning at 6 AM the woman awoke to find the man standing over her bed with a knife. He assaulted and raped her, and fled the scene. She called the police and reported the crime. In the next following weeks, she was contacted by an anonymous caller who she immediately claimed was the rapist because she recognized his voice. He called several times and the police were able to record a few of the calls. The Investigation The victim was able to help produce a composite sketch of the suspect. David Shawn Pope became a suspect after police saw him around the area and thought he looked similar to the sketch. Pope was presented to the victim multiple times in a photo lineup, along with other similar looking males and no identification was made. After there was no identification, those six peop...